Main Body

CHAPTER 6: CHILDREN AND THE LAW

Chapter Objectives

  1. Summarize the recent history of children as witnesses.
  2. Outline the strengths and weaknesses of children as witnesses.
  3. Outline the recommendations for interviewing child witnesses.
  4. Explain how courts determine the competence of children as witnesses in criminal court.
  5. Explain why credibility of children is important and how it is evaluated.
  6. Explain how Canadian criminal courts accommodate child witnesses.

 

Very young children are witnesses in criminal court, usually as complainants. How professionals and nonprofessionals talk to children about allegations can have a profound effect on the quality of children’s reports. The goals must be to maximize the amount of correct information reported about the allegations and to minimize the amount of incorrect information reported. Researchers have identified several ways not to talk to children and these are briefly introduced in the section on children’s suggestibility. Researchers have also studied various techniques that advance the goals of the forensic system including conducting all interviews according to the characteristics of a good interview, having children promise to tell the truth, and allowing children to testify with testimonial aids.

We begin this chapter with case studies that represent the best and the worst of children’s testimony in criminal court. Case Study 6.1 summarizes two cases, both of which illustrate how remarkably accurate children’s reports can be. Case Study 6.2 illustrates how remarkably inaccurate children can be when interviewed in ways that are biased. The message students should take from this second case study is not how suggestible and unreliable children are, but how untrained interviewers who use shockingly biased techniques can induce children to report events that are complex, detailed, emotionally charged . . . and false!

CASE STUDY 6.1. Children can be remarkable witnesses

Orbach and Lamb (1999) described the case of a 13-year old victim who was in the bathroom listening to music when her grandfather arrived. His plan was to sexually abuse her, another incident in a long series of sexual assaults that had been occurring for two years. Before the abuse began, the victim pushed the “record” button on the tape deck, providing an audio record of the entire incident. After the assault, the victim gave the audiotape to her mother, and the police were contacted. The perpetrator was interviewed by the police two days after the incident. This is a rare case where there was independent evidence of details of the abuse that could be used to assess the accuracy of the child’s report. The victim’s report contained 189 details; 51 percent were corroborated on the tape, and almost all of the corroborated details were central (98 percent). Most of the details that were not corroborated described actions that could not be confirmed based on an audiotape. Only one detail reported by the victim was contradicted by other evidence. The victim’s report was remarkably complete and accurate.
Another example of how extraordinarily well children can recall and report their victimization was described by Jones and Krugman (1986). Susie, a three-year-old child, was abducted outside her home. She was sexually assaulted, thrown down an outhouse toilet, and left for dead. Seventy hours after her abduction she was discovered and hospitalized. Five days after her abduction she was interviewed by the police. She described a sexual assault and identified the perpetrator from two different lineups. Fourteen days after her abduction, she was presented with a lineup that did not contain the suspect; she was told that the “bad man” was in the lineup. Susie rejected all members of the lineup, reporting that the “bad man” was not there. Immediately after Susie rejected the lineup, the perpetrator’s photo was added and she was asked again to identify the “bad man.” Once again, she identified the perpetrator. Eventually, the perpetrator made a full confession and described his sexual assault of Susie just as she had described it to the police.

 

CASE STUDY 6.2. How NOT to interview children

The saga began in 1991 in Saskatchewan when a three-year-old said, in response to her mother’s query about her “red bum,” that “Ravis” had “poked” her with a “pink rope.” “Ravis” was the child’s name for Travis, the son of her daycare provider. The comment launched a massive investigation of the daycare that led to multiple charges against nine people: the daycare providers, Linda and Ron Sterling; their son, Travis; a minor living in the Sterling home at the time; and five members of the local police force, John Popwich, Jim Elstad, Edward Revesz, Darryl Ford, and Darren Sabourin.

After extensive and repeated highly suggestive questioning, the children from the daycare reported heinous and ritualistic abuse. The children said they were made to urinate in the mouths of the defendants; stripped naked, placed in cages, then suspended from the ceiling; injected with a clear liquid; placed in a freezer; made to have sex with each other; subjected to anal intercourse; and forced to watch animal torture and human sacrifice. Notwithstanding the horrific nature of the allegations, there was absolutely no physical evidence to support them—in fact, there was no other indication that anything out of the ordinary was happening at the daycare operated by the Sterlings. The case rested solely on the reports made by the children.
So what went wrong with the investigation? Just about everything. To begin, when the first allegation was made, the police interviewed other daycare children and their families about possible child sexual abuse. The investigation quickly expanded to include any child with whom the Sterlings could have had contact. A theme quickly developed in the questions asked by police, a presumption that the Sterlings were bad people who did very bad things to children. This stereotype was maintained and fuelled through community meetings, interviews with parents and children, and therapy sessions with the children. Soon the community was on fire with allegations of satanic ritualistic abuse of children.
Details of the allegations were disclosed to children, parents, and the community in several different ways. For example, when investigators determined that some abuse had occurred in a “Devil Church” that was not part of the Sterling home (it is not clear how this idea developed), members of the community became involved in locating it. A local resident who was not otherwise associated with the investigation identified a building just outside town as a candidate to be the Devil Church. The chief of police drove two of the children and their parents to the location and asked them to confirm it was the Devil Church. The children complied. Pictures were taken of the inside and outside of the building and shown to other children, and the church was described as the place where terrible things happened to children.
The police held public meetings to update the community on the state of the investigation. Parents of children who might have been abused were routinely told of new details revealed by the children. In most cases, the parents discussed the details of the allegations with their children. Children were also shown pictures of props and of other people who might have been involved in the alleged abuse.
The police interviewed the children repeatedly; some were interviewed more than a dozen times. The primary objective of the interviews was to confirm the events that police officers believed to be true. The questions were strongly leading in several ways: the alleged perpetrators were described as bad people who did bad things to children; children were told that others had disclosed, they were given details of the other disclosures, and they were told that they should disclose too; children were told that disclosure would help other children and would allow the perpetrators to get the help they desperately needed; questions were repeated—in at least one interview 19 times—until a disclosure was produced; after a disclosure, the question was not repeated; to help them remember the abuse, children were encouraged to imagine and pretend; and they were given rewards for disclosing abuse—for example, children were taken to see a police dog, they were told how brave they were for making a disclosure, and/or they were permitted to leave the interview after a disclosure.
During the investigation, the children were in therapy. Two of the children were involved in intense play therapy with a therapist who had been briefed by the police about the nature of the allegations. The therapist for two children employed a “disclosure” technique that involved repeated requests to disclose the abuse. Another child was treated with a “good guy/bad guy” play approach in which the Sterlings were always cast as the bad guys.
The charges against the five police officers were eventually stayed. The case of Ron, Linda, and Travis Sterling went to trial and was heard by a judge and jury. The trial lasted five months. Ron and Linda Sterling were acquitted. Their son, Travis Sterling, was convicted of eight charges against six children and sentenced to an aggregate term of five years in prison. The other minor in the Sterling home also went to trial in 1993 and was convicted of seven sex-related offences.
The convictions of Travis Sterling and the other minor were appealed. In 1995, the Saskatchewan Court of Appeal overturned seven of the eight convictions of Travis Sterling, entering acquittals for them. The only conviction that was upheld concerned the one related to the first child’s report that “Ravis” poked her with a pink rope. In quashing the other convictions, the court held that the interviews with the children were so flawed that the children’s reports, the only evidence at trial, could not be relied on. All seven convictions against the other minor were overturned.
Most of the accused persons launched malicious prosecution civil suits. In 2002, after ten long years of legal haggling, John Popwich won his malicious prosecution suit; he was given an official apology from the government and a $1.3-million settlement. In 2004, the province agreed to pay $925,000 to Ron and Linda Sterling and the minor who was charged at the same time. In 2005, Darren Sabourin was awarded $150,000. In 2006, Jim Elstad was awarded $285,000 and Edward Revesz was awarded $175,000. Darryl Ford, apparently, did not file a civil suit. This case has cost more than$2.8 million in malicious prosecution damages alone.
February 12, 2003, CBC News website, www.cbc.ca/fifth/martin/popowich.html; The Star-Phoenix, “Martensville Scandal,” Saskatoon Star-Phoenix, June 24, 2006, www.canada.com/saskatoonstarphoenix/ news/weekend_extra/story.html?id=eec1a825-874448a7-943f-b6319f26f578&k=52612; Government of Saskatchewan, “Settlements Reached in Martensville Lawsuits,” news release, March 7, 2006, www.gov.sk.ca/news?newsId=e6e89e6a-89c9-4c71-a709-56fa38438e37.Sources: “The Satanic Sex Scandal: John Popowich,” background to “Hell to Pay,” an episode of The Fifth Estate,

 

Learning Objective 6.1

Summarize the recent history of children as witnesses.

recent complaint doctrine a law that was in force in Canada until 1983, which stated that in all cases involving sexual offences, if the victim did not complain at the first available opportunity, the trier of fact was required to assume that the complainant was not credible or had consented to having sex

(Recent) History of Child Witnesses

“Before 1982, sexual offences involving children were virtually impossible to prosecute to conviction” according to Van Tongeren and Dauns (2001, p. 148). Although there were no written laws prohibiting children from testifying in criminal court, there were at least three significant obstacles to successful prosecution. Until 1983, the recent complaint doctrine was in effect in Canada (see Criminal Code, 1985, s. 275). According to this law, if a victim of sexual assault, whether a child or an adult, did not complain of the assault at the first available opportunity, the trier of fact was required to assume that the complainant was not credible or had consented to having sex. This doctrine had a profound impact on prosecutions involving child sexual abuse because up to twothirds of children delay reporting their sexual abuse (London, Bruck, Ceci, & Shuman, 2005).

INSIGHT 6.1. RECENT COMPLAINT DOCTRINE

In R. v. D.D. (2000), the Supreme Court of Canada provided the following ‘archaic passage’ describing the complaint doctrine:

When therefore a virgin has been so deflowered and overpowered, against the peace of the lord the king, forthwith and while the act is fresh she ought to repair with hue and cry to the neighboring vills and there display to honest men the injury done to her, the blood and her dress stained with blood, and the tearing of her dress; and so she ought to go to the provost of the hundred and to the serjeant of the lord the king and to the coroners and to the viscount and make her appeal at the first county court.

By the year 2000, most common-law countries had disposed of the recent complaint doctrine. Some countries replaced it with a judicial direction that stated that delayed reporting could not be taken as evidence that the complainant was not credible or had consented to having sex. (Lewis, 2006)

 

Second, until 1988 there was a requirement that courts be warned of the dangers of convicting on the uncorroborated evidence of an unsworn child (Criminal Code, s. 659). An unsworn child is one who testifies on a promise to tell the truth rather than on an oath or affirmation. This rule was strict and required an acquittal if there was no independent corroborating evidence, even if the trier of fact was otherwise convinced of the truth of the charge (Bala, 1990). This rule was particularly oppressive for child sexual assault victims because often in these cases there is no corroborative evidence (Bala, 1999; Sas & Cunningham, 1995), and because young children often testify as unsworn witnesses. In fact, in Canada today all children under 14 years of age testify as unsworn witnesses in criminal court.

INSIGHT 6.2. Attitudes towards child witnesses

John Wigmore is one of the most influential legal scholars of the 20th century. His 1940 treatise, often called ‘Wigmore on Evidence’, is considered on of the greatest books on law. In fact, it is cited often by the Supreme Court of Canada because it is an accurate and eloquent statement of the law. Wigmore’s description of the attitude towards women’s and children’s allegations of sexual assault is not positive. He wrote:
Modern psychiatrists have amply studied the behaviour of errant young girls and women coming before the courts in all sorts of cases. Their psychic complexes are multifarious, distorted partly by inherent defects, partly by diseased derangements or abnormal instincts, partly by bad social environment, partly by temporary psychological or emotional conditions. One form taken by these complexes is that of contriving false charges of sexual offences by men (cited in Bala, 1990, p.15)

The third barrier was one of attitude. There was a pervasive belief that the evidence of children was inherently unreliable, particularly in allegations of sexual assault. By the late 1980s the recent complaint doctrine and the requirement for corroboration of unsworn children’s evidence had been done away with. Beginning in about the mid-1980s there was a dramatic increase in research on the issue of children as witnesses, and the number of children entering criminal courts as witnesses increased dramatically. Ceci and Bruck (1995) explain this as a consequence of several changes: (a) a wider admissibility of expert psychological testimony on issues of eyewitness accuracy (at least in the United States); the sociopolitical zeitgeist of the late 1960s that encouraged social scientists to focus more on the application of their research—the need for applied research was particularly acute when it could help vulnerable members of society, such as children; (c) the “sudden” awareness of the prevalence of crimes against children (e.g., the Badgley Report, released in 1984, investigated sexual offences against children in Canada); and (d) the changes in law that made it possible for children to be witnesses in criminal court.

Children’s Suggestibility

Learning Objective 6.2

Outline the strengths and weaknesses of children as witnesses.

children’s suggestibility the degree to which social and psychological factors affect the way children remember, retrieve, and report details about the past (Ceci & Bruck, 1993)

During the mid- to late 1980s there was a dramatic increase in studies on children’s suggestibility, defined by Ceci and Bruck (1993) as “the degree to which encoding, storage, retrieval, and reporting of events can be influenced by a range of social and psychological factors” (p. 404). In other words, children will sometimes report false details or false events when social and psychological factors affect the way they remember, retrieve, and report details about the past. Part of the impetus for this research was the large number of criminal cases involving allegations of child sexual abuse. These cases raised serious concerns about techniques used to interview children and the possibility that some techniques could lead to false reports.

We often think of suggestibility as a consequence of leading questions, and leading questions can certainly lead to suggestibility effects in children and adults. However, this example does not describe the wide range of circumstances that can lead to suggestibility effects (Ceci & Bruck, 2006). When children are exposed to biases and/or preconceived notions about what happened, suggestibility effects can be expected among some children. Biases can be expressed to children in a variety of ways. For example, direct accusations that someone did something bad to a child can have profound effects on the child’s report (Thompson, ClarkeStewart, & Lepore, 1997). However, even apparently subtle biases can have significant effects.
In the famous Sam Stone study, Sam Stone visited a daycare with children aged 3 to 4 and 5 to 6 years old (Leichtman & Ceci, 1995). Sam entered the daycare, walked around, commented on the book being read, waved goodbye, and left. Some of the children had been told once a week for four weeks before Sam’s visit that he was a nice person but kind of bumbling and clumsy (stereotype). The same children were also told once a week for four weeks after Sam’s visit that he ripped a book and soiled a teddy bear during his visit (suggestion). Other children, who made up a control group, were not given the stereotype or suggestions. Ten weeks after Sam’s visit the children were given a memory interview that began with free recall—an open-ended request to report everything that happened during Sam Stone’s visit. Children who did not mention a soiled teddy bear or a ripped book in free recall were asked about these misdeeds in the next phase of the interview—probed recall. For instance, children who did not mention the ripped book in free recall were asked, “I heard something about a book. Do you know anything about that?” Children who mentioned either of the misdeeds in free or probed recall were gently challenged: “Did you see him do it?” If a child responded “Yes,” the interviewer challenged the child more strongly by asking, “You didn’t really see it, did you?” Results are illustrated in figures 6.1a and 6.1b (see Canvas).

Of the children in the control group, none reported that Sam committed a misdeed (soiled a teddy bear or ripped a book) in response to the free recall question. Some of the children reported at least one misdeed in response to the probed recall, but only 2.5 percent of the younger children (and none of the older children) maintained the report when the stronger challenge was administered. Contrast this with responses from children who were given the stereotype before Sam’s visit and the suggestions afterward. A substantial minority of these children made at least one erroneous claim in free recall. In response to probed recall, more younger than older children maintained that Sam had soiled a teddy bear and/or ripped a book, even when strongly challenged. Clearly, the combination of stereotypes and suggestions was powerful and had a much stronger effect on younger children than older children.

Several other factors have been shown to have a negative effect on children’s reports, and we’ll review a subset of factors to introduce the student to this literature. In some studies, interviewers set a tone that encouraged and selectively rewarded disclosure of wrongdoing (Garven, Wood, & Malpass, 2000; Goodman, Batterman-Faunce, Schaaf, & Kenney, 2002). Early in the interview, children in the Goodman et al. study were asked if they “were afraid to tell” and were reassured, “You will feel better once you’ve told.” In Garven et al. children were rewarded for making a false disclosure (e.g., “Great. You’re doing excellent now”) and discouraged from denying the wrongdoing. In both studies, a number of children reported misdeeds that had not occurred.

Another powerful technique that can elicit false reports from children is peer influence (for an excellent review of this new area of research see Principe & Schindewolf, 2012). In one of the first demonstrations of the power of peer influence (Principe & Ceci, 2002), preschoolers went on an “archaeological dig.” During the dig, some children in each class observed a research assistant ruin two archaeological props; other children from the same classes did not observe destruction of the props. To encourage children who observed the misdeeds to talk to their classmates, the destruction of the props was staged to be very dramatic. A third group of children who went on the same archaeological dig did not observe the misdeeds and were not exposed to the peer influence (this was the control condition). On three separate occasions after the archaeological dig, half of the children in each observation group were questioned suggestively about the destruction of the two props. Approximately four weeks after the dig, all children were interviewed in a neutral way about what they had seen. Not surprisingly, suggestive questioning alone led to false reports; among children in the control condition, those who were questioned suggestively were more likely to report having seen the misdeeds than children who were not questioned suggestively. The most dramatic effect was among children who were questioned suggestively and subjected to peer influence. In this group, children who had not witnessed the misdeeds were as likely as children who had witnessed them to report that the misdeeds had occurred during their archaeological dig.

The technique of encouraging a child to imagine or to pretend that something happened (e.g., “Try to make a picture of it in your head” and “What do you think you would have been wearing?”) has not been the subject of the same level of carefully controlled research as other suggestive interviewing techniques. However, Wood and Garven (2000) offer three reasons to suspect that inviting a child to play, pretend, or speculate can lead to false allegations: clinical observation, which shows that inviting a person to speculate can lead to false reports; the legal principle that precludes witnesses from speculating; and research on false memories, which finds that inviting a person to imagine or speculate can lead to false memories. In

Ceci, Loftus, Leichtman, and Bruck (1994), some children reported having experienced very complex and emotionally charged events (e.g., going to the hospital for stitches) after they were invited to imagine what the event would have been like and were told that their parents said the event really had happened. Until recently, it has been generally accepted that younger children are more suggestible than older children and adults. This is usually true; however, there is at least one important exception (for an excellent review of this research see Brainerd & Reyna, 2012). One of the first demonstrations that older children can be more suggestible than younger children comes from Brainerd, Reyna, and Forrest (2002). Children and adults heard a list of thematically related words—for example, bed, rest, awake, snooze, doze, and nap—and then were asked to recall the words. The researchers measured the number of list words recalled and the number of intrusions—thematically related words that were NOT on the list, such as sleep in this example. There was a very clear developmental improvement in the amount of correct information recalled, and there was also an increase across age in the number of intrusions reported. Adults were more likely than children to recall thematically related words that were not presented. In Connolly and Price (2006), 4-to 5-year-olds and 6-to 7-year-olds played a game four times with some thematically related variation across experiences. After the last play session, children were given thematically related misleading information about some of the details of the last play session. Older children were more suggestible than younger children. It seems that when experienced items are thematically related, and when suggestions are members of the same category, older children and adults are more suggestible than younger children. Brainerd et al. (2002) reasoned that it is harder for younger children to identify the relationship between items, so they are less likely to falsely report items that are thematically related but not experienced.

In most of the research on children’s suggestibility, children are exposed to one or two inappropriate interviewing techniques. In R. v. Sterling (described in Case Study 6.2), children were exposed to multiple and repeated suggestive techniques, so it is not surprising that their reports were bizarre and, according to the Saskatchewan Court of Appeal, false. Just as a poorly conducted interview can lead to false reports from children, a well-conducted interview can lead to reports that are comprehensive, accurate, and sufficient to convict a perpetrator.

Interviews with Children

When it is suspected that a crime has been committed against a child, at least three questions arise: Is the child in need of protection for the purposes of child protection legislation? Should a criminal charge be laid? Is the child in need of therapy? The answers to these questions usually must come from the child’s report of events, often in the context of a forensic interview. A good forensic interview should have as its core objectives (1) to identify the occurrence of child maltreatment and (2) to identify when an allegation or suspicion of maltreatment is false. The task of interviewing a child, particularly a young child, about suspected maltreatment is daunting. A forensic interviewer must remain cognizant of the possibility of false allegations and, at the same time, the reluctance/inability of some children to provide forensically adequate reports. The seemingly competing goals combined with the special challenges associated with talking to young children may lead (and have led) some interviewers to use techniques that seriously compromise the accuracy and the perceived credibility of the child’s report.

Learning Objective 6.3

Outline the recommendations for interviewing child witnesses.

commission errors reporting false details

Characteristics of a Good Interview

According to Saywitz and Camparo (2009), “there is remarkable consensus among clinicians and researchers on a wide range of techniques and general guidelines for interviewing children in a forensic setting from a developmental perspective” (p. 104). These principles apply to all interviews with children, not just formal interviews conducted by professionals. We are able to recommend guidelines to police and other forensic interviewers, but what about all of the informal “discussions” with parents, peers, and other interested persons? The effect these informal discussions can have on a child’s report can be profound— and wholly unknown (Principe & Schindewolf, 2012).

Setting of the Interview

Poole and Lamb (2002) suggest that the location of the interview should be child-friendly but not distracting. It takes time for children to warm up to a new environment, and too much stimuli in the environment may extend the time and distract the child.

Provide a Supportive Environment

There is good empirical evidence that non-contingent social support has a positive effect on children’s resistance to suggestions and their accuracy, particularly if there is a delay between the event and the interview (Bottoms, Quas, & Davis, 2007; Carter, Bottoms, & Levine, 1996; Davis & Bottoms, 2002). Such support has been defined as warm vocal tones, supportive eye contact, frequent smiling, actions that build rapport, and a relaxed body posture (as demonstrated in Figure 6.2) Carter et al. (1996) reported that, in response to direct questions, 5-to 7-year-olds were more accurate, made fewer commission errors (that is, they reported fewer false details), and were less suggestible in a supportive interviewer condition than they were in a non-supportive interviewer condition. Similarly, Teoh & Lamb (2013) reported a positive relationship between interviewer support and children’s informativeness in a sample of 75 forensic interviews with children who reported having been sexually abused.

Build Rapport in a Practice Interview

As part of the rapport-building stage, interviewers should give the child a practice interview, in which the child has an opportunity to describe a non-threatening past event in detail. Interviewers should ask about the past event in the same way they will ask the child about the alleged maltreatment. The practice interview serves a variety of objectives. First, it helps to build rapport. Second, it gives interviewers a chance to observe the child’s linguistic competence. This will help interviewers set their own linguistic style. It also lets interviewers know if there is a language shift when the child talks about the alleged abuse (i.e., does the child revert to “baby language” or use language that is too maturesounding) and allows them to investigate reasons for the shift. Third, it lets the child know how they will be interviewed during the substantive portion of the interview and how much information the interviewer wants to hear. Finally, it gives the child an opportunity to practise responding to open questions about past events.

As should be clear from this list of objectives, it is important for the child to spend a considerable amount of time talking during the practice interview. This is encouraged by asking open prompts. If the interviewer asks too many questions, it may have a negative effect on the amount of information provided by younger children during the substantive phase of the interview. To study the effects of using open versus closed prompts in the rapport-building stage, Sternberg et al. (1997) reviewed 51 interviews investigating allegations of child sexual abuse with children between the ages of 4.5 and 12.9. During the rapport-building stage, interviewers used either open prompts or closed questions. The substantive part of the interview was similar for all children. In response to the first substantive question, children who had been given open prompts during the practice interview provided 2.5 times more information than children given closed prompts during practice, and they continued to provide more information in response to open prompts throughout the interview. Teoh and Lamb (2010) studied 75 forensic interviews with children in Malaysia. They found that the number of prompts in the rapport-building phase was negatively correlated with the informativeness of 5-to 7-year-olds, but it showed no relation to the informativeness of 8to 12-year-olds or 13to 15-year-olds (see also Teoh & Lamb, 2013). Roberts, Lamb, and Sternberg (2004) replicated this effect with a lab-based study. Children whose practice interview contained primarily open prompts provided more correct information about an earlier play session than children whose practice interview contained primarily closed prompts. This suggests that the practice interview is important and should not contain too many prompts from the interviewer.

open prompts prompts or questions that allow a response that is minimally constrained by the question (e.g., “tell me more about that”)

closed prompts prompts or questions that constrain the possible responses (e.g., “Who was driving the car?” or “Was it Tuesday or Wednesday?”)

omission errors failing to report details

Set the Ground Rules

Children should be told to say only what really happened but to report everything that actually happened. There is good empirical evidence that a promise to tell the truth has truth-promoting effects with children—we will talk about this later in this chapter when we discuss children’s competence. Although children should only report what they remember happened, they should not edit information. Interviewers must let children know they should report everything, even if the details are partial or seem unimportant.

Interviewers should empower children to become active participants in the interview. Interviewers should tell them to ask for clarification if they do not understand, to say “I don’t know” or “I don’t remember” if that is the appropriate response, and to correct the interviewer if something is not right. It is also recommended that they give children a chance to practise using these responses. For instance, the interviewer could ask the child, “When is my birthday?” The correct answer is “I don’t know,” and the child should be encouraged to respond that way. This may seem obvious to us as adults, but children often need permission, encouragement, and practice to use these responses when talking to adults—particularly adults in positions of authority.

There is mixed data on the efficacy of telling children to say “I don’t know” and “I don’t remember” if that is the appropriate response. Poole and Lamb (2002) reviewed several studies and reported that sometimes the instruction increased the number of “I don’t know” responses, and sometimes the instructions were not helpful with younger children. It is not the case that such instructions are sometimes helpful and sometimes innocuous; some researchers have found that they can actually increase omission errors, or the failure to report details.

If too much emphasis is placed on saying “I don’t know” or “I don’t understand” without also training the children when a substantive answer is appropriate, children may overuse the responses as a way of demonstrating how well they are following instructions. Saywitz and Moan-Hardie (1994) provided 7-year-olds with extensive training on the “I don’t know” response and measured the effect on correct and incorrect responses in a suggestibility study. In one experiment they found that the instruction decreased the number of incorrect responses, but it also decreased the number of correct responses. In a second experiment, children were given similar training but were also given extensive training on how to respond substantively if they were able to do so.

In this study, training decreased the number of incorrect responses but did not affect the number of correct responses. Gee, Gregory, and Pipe (1999) reported similar data. Half of the 9-to 10-year-olds and 11-to 13-year-olds in their study were instructed on the importance of saying “I don’t know,” and half were given no such instruction. All children were then asked misleading and non-misleading questions about a recent visit to an interactive museum. Children who had been given instructions committed fewer errors but also reported fewer correct details. In another experiment, when 9-to 10-year-old children were also instructed on the importance of providing an answer if possible, children who had been instructed committed fewer errors, and there was no effect on correct details reported.

Remain Objective and Neutral

Interviewers should remain objective and neutral with respect to the truth of the allegations they are investigating. Biased interviewers can and often do influence the interview in such a way that they are more likely to obtain responses that are consistent with their hypotheses. In research by White, Leichtman, and Ceci (1997), preschool children were interviewed by a trained social worker or an elementary school teacher about a Simon Says game that they had played about three weeks earlier. The interviewers were exposed to some correct and some incorrect information prior to talking to the children. As expected, the interviewers asked misleading questions that were consistent with what they believed had happened, and some children acquiesced to the suggestions. Similarly, Goodman, Sharma, Thomas, and Considine (1995) found that, in response to free-recall prompts, preschoolers reported more inaccurate information when the interviewer had been biased (i.e., informed of false details before the interview) than when the interviewer had not been biased.

Effects of interviewer bias are not restricted to interviews with professionals; they have also been observed when misled parents interview their children (Principe, DiPuppo, & Gammel, 2011). In this study, mothers were asked to discuss with their child a magic show their child had recently attended. Some mothers were told that a rabbit that was supposed to be in the magic show “may” have escaped from its cage (a rabbit had not escaped), while other mothers were not misled. The children of mothers who were misled were far more likely to report that the rabbit had escaped from its cage and that they saw it than children of mothers who were not misled. This is a powerful example of how a rather minor and tentative suggestion to the interviewer can have a dramatic effect on children’s reports.

One mechanism that could lead to inaccurate reports when the interviewer is misled is the use of suggestive questions, but that is not the only one. Interviewers may behave in ways that let the child know what is expected; they may nod or smile when the child says something that is consistent with their hypothesis, and the child may pick up on these subtle cues and respond in the way interviewers expect. Also, interviewers are more likely to recall and accept statements from the child that are consistent with their hypothesis.

Avoid Suggestions

The topic of suggestibility was discussed in more detail earlier in this chapter. At this point it is enough to say that suggestive questions are dangerous and may lead to reports that are inaccurate or are judged to be inaccurate.

Use Appropriate Question Format

Interviewers should use open-ended questions and encourage the child to elaborate. There is convincing evidence that responses to open-ended questions are more accurate than responses to specific questions. The substantive portion of the interview should start with an open invitation (e.g., “Can you tell me why you are here today?”). If the child provides a partial disclosure, the interviewer should use an open prompt to invite the child to say more (e.g., “Tell me everything that happened”) followed by open-ended prompts as appropriate (e.g., “What happened next?”). If the initial invitation does not lead to even a partial disclosure, the interviewer must decide whether to introduce the topic with more directive questions (e.g., “Has anything happened to you that you would like to tell me about?”) or to conduct the interview later.

When it appears that the child has said all that he or she can recall in response to open prompts, there may still be details missing from the report. In this case, some specific prompts may be necessary. The interviewer should begin with “wh” questions (who, what, where, when, but not why). Directive questions can be leading, so it is important to restrict direct questions to details already raised by the child (e.g., if the child mentioned a car, the interviewer may ask “What colour was the car?” or “Where did you go in the car?”).

Although leading questions should be avoided, they are sometimes necessary to obtain all forensically relevant information from a child. When they are necessary, interviewers should avoid the following circumstances, which may heighten the negative effect of suggestive questions (Perona, Bottoms, & Sorenson, 2006):

  • Don’t ask leading questions repeatedly in the same interview or over a long period of time.
  • Make sure they are not asked by a biased, an emotionally intimidating, or a high-status interviewer.
  • Don’t ask leading questions in a coercive way (e.g., “You can play with the toy after you tell me what happened”).
  • Don’t ask leading questions about peripheral, rather than central, details.
  • Don’t use developmentally inappropriate language.
  • Don’t ask very leading questions.

Some question formats should be avoided at any time, including coercive statements (“You can leave as soon as you tell me what happened”), tacked-on questions that ask for verification (“He hurt you, didn’t he?”), negative terms (“Didn’t he hurt you?”), suppositional questions (“Was she mad when that happened?”), and multiple-choice questions (“Was mom or dad in the house?”).

Closing

Interviewers should take care to close the interview well: prepare children for the next step, thank them for their effort (not for what was said), and invite questions.

Repeated Events

Most of the research described in this chapter concerns children’s reports of a unique event. However, about half of all allegations of sexual offences against children involve repeated abuse (Connolly & Read, 2006; Sas, 1993; Sas & Cunningham, 1995). Since the late 1990s, researchers have been studying issues related to reports of repeated abuse. Excellent reviews of research on the theory of children’s memory for repeated events have been done by Fivush (2002) as well as Hudson and Mayhew (2009). The purpose of this section is to apply the research to child witnesses.

In the areas of suggestibility and perceived credibility, children who report repeated abuse are sometimes disadvantaged compared to children who report abuse that occurred once. Those who experience an event repeatedly are likely to be more suggestible, particularly if suggestions are consistent with the theme of the events (Roberts & Powell, 2006) and if memory is measured by recognition rather than recall (Connolly & Lindsay, 2001; Powell & Roberts, 2002; but see Price & Connolly, 2004).

Children who report an instance of a repeated event are seen to be less credible than children who report a unique event when intuitive judgments are made (as is often the case when investigators, interviewers, and triers of fact assess children’s credibility; Connolly, Price, Lavoie, & Gordon, 2008), but they are more credible than single-event children when credibility is assessed using a systematic analysis of verbal content (known as Criterion-Based Content Analysis, which is discussed in the section on credibility) (Blandon-Gitlan, Pezdek, Rogers, & Brodie, 2005; Pezdek et al., 2004; Strömwall, Bengtsson, Leander, & Granhag, 2004).

There is a dearth of research on interviewing children who report repeated abuse. Most recommendations for interviews come from research on children’s memory for single events. However, principles that apply to good forensic interviewing of children who report a single episode of abuse can also apply to children who report repeated abuse (Hershkowitz, 2001), with two possible modifications. If it is known before the interview begins that repeated abuse is suspected, Brubacher, Roberts, & Powell (2011) recommend that the subject of the practice interview be an instance of a benign repeated event. One purpose of a practice interview is to provide the child with an opportunity to practise responding to the kinds of questions that will be asked during the substantive portion of the interview, so if the substantive interview will focus on reporting details of an instance of a repeated event, it would be helpful to include that context in practice. Several forensic interview protocols recommend that children be directed to report a particular instance that they remember best (Hershkowitz, 2002; Lamb, Orbach, Hershkowitz, Esplin, & Horowitz, 2007). A particular instance is investigated because information that is obtained during the interview will inform the charge, and that charge must be particular enough for the accused to raise a defence (Brodie v. The King, 1936; Connolly, Price, & Gordon, 2009; Guadagno, Powell, & Wright, 2006).

Learning Objective 6.4

Explain how courts determine the competence of children as witnesses in criminal court.

competence a question of admissibility of testimony

truth-lie discussion an examination into a child’s understanding of concepts like “truth,” “lie,” “promise,” and the morality of telling the truth; this is done in some jurisdictions before a child is permitted to testify in court


Competence of Child Witnesses

In the next two sections we discuss competence and credibility. It is important to keep these two concepts distinct. Competence is concerned with whether a witness is allowed to testify in court (admissibility), while credibility is concerned with how much emphasis (weight) the trier of fact places on the testimony after it has been admitted.

In Canada until January 2, 2006, children (i.e., people under the age of 14 years) had to pass a competency examination before they were allowed to testify in criminal courts. This meant that children were asked questions about concepts associated with telling the truth, such as their understanding of concepts like “truth,” “lie,” “promise,” and the morality of telling the truth (this is known as a truth-lie discussion). If the judge decided that a child understood these concepts and could understand and respond to questions, that child would, typically, be directed to promise to tell the truth and be allowed to testify. Today in Canada, persons under the age of 14 years are presumed competent to testify and shall do so after promising to tell the truth. A truth-lie discussion is prohibited, although a child’s ability to understand and respond to questions may be examined if the court is convinced that there is an issue as to competence.

In several jurisdictions the truth-lie discussion has been abolished (it has disappeared in Canada, England, New Zealand, and Scotland, as well as in all but two states in Australia), but it does still have a place in the forensic process. Most states in the United States continue to require that a judge conduct a truth-lie competence inquiry before a child is permitted to testify in court. Further, in jurisdictions where a truth-lie inquiry is not required by law, one may be conducted during forensic interviews with children (Bala, Paetsch, Bertrand, & Thomas, 2010).

When a truth-lie inquiry is required or allowed, it typically begins with questions about the difference between truth and lies, and about the nature of a promise, and ends with the child promising to tell the truth. It is useful to think about this as a two-step process: the truth-lie discussion (TL discussion), and then the promise to tell the truth. In order to determine if both a TL discussion and a promise to tell the truth are necessary, researchers had to develop procedures by which they could draw false statements from children. Two methods are common: a temptation paradigm and a transgression paradigm. In temptation paradigms, children play with a toy that includes hidden or secret parts. Sometime during the game the experimenter is called out of the room, and the child is asked not to peek or play until the experimenter returns and the game resumes; in spite of this, a substantial number of children peek or play while the experimenter is out of the room. When the experimenter returns, the children are asked if they peeked. In transgression paradigms, the child is in a room with an adult confederate when a transgression occurs (committed by the adult or both the adult and the child). The adult asks the child to not tell anyone what happened. The experimenter returns and asks the child what happened.

Talwar, Lee, Bala, and Lindsay (2002) used a temptation paradigm with 3-to 7-year-old children (Experiment 3). Half of the children engaged in a TL discussion (without being asked to promise to tell the truth) before they were asked if they peeked; the other children promised to tell the truth before being asked if they peeked. Fewer children who had promised to tell the truth lied compared to children in the TL discussion condition. Evans and Lee (2010) also found that a promise to tell the truth, but not a TL discussion, reduced lying behaviour among 8-to 16-year-olds.

In the context of temptation paradigms, when children were asked if they committed a transgression, a TL discussion did not promote truth-telling behaviour, but a promise to tell the truth did. A similar effect was found by Lyon, Saywitz, Kaplan, and Dorado (2001), who used a transgression paradigm with maltreated children. While the experimenter was out of the room, 6-and 7-year-olds with documented histories of maltreatment were enticed to play with the toy by a confederate, who said, “We might get into trouble if anyone found out.” During a subsequent interview, half of the children were asked to promise to tell the truth and half were not (the control group). About 50 percent of the children in the control group lied about playing with the toy, but only 20 percent of the children who promised to tell the truth lied.

Lyon and colleagues have continued to study children with documented histories of maltreatment. In one study (Lyon & Dorado, 2008) they used a transgression paradigm with 5to 7-year-old children. A confederate joined the child and together they played with the forbidden toy while the experimenter was out of the room. When the experimenter returned, one-third of the children were asked to promise to tell the truth, one-third of the children were assured that they would not get into trouble if they told the truth and were asked to promise to tell the truth, and one-third of the children were given no instructions before the interview began (the control group). Honesty scores were higher in both groups that had promised to tell the truth than in the control group. This demonstrates that a promise to tell the truth also promotes honesty when asked about another person’s transgression.

Talwar, Lee, Bala, and Lindsay (2004) also demonstrated the truth-promoting effect of a promise to tell the truth when the incident under investigation is a parent’s transgression. In their study, parents of children between the ages of 3 and 11 accidently “broke” a puppet while the experimenter was out of the room. All parents asked their child not to tell the experimenter how the puppet got broken because the parent might get into trouble. When the experimenter returned, the child was asked four questions about how the puppet got broken. A second experimenter arrived, asked half of the children to promise to tell the truth, and asked all children the four questions again. Of the children who lied in the first interview, those who promised to tell the truth before the second interview were more likely to change their response to an honest answer than children who did not promise to tell the truth.

This research suggests that a formal competency inquiry seems unnecessary, but a promise to tell the truth is important across a range of ages, transgressions, and transgressors.

Learning Objective 6.5

Explain why credibility of children is important and how it is evaluated.
credibility the emphasis or weight the trier of fact places on an individual’s testimony

Credibility of Children

In many cases that involve children as complainants, findings of credibility are determinative at each stage of the process. That is, the first person to whom the child discloses must believe the report is credible before contacting the police; the police must believe the report is credible before launching a full investigation; the Crown must believe the report is credible before deciding to go to trial; and the trier of fact must believe that the report is credible before convicting the accused. Because there is rarely any corroborating evidence, these decisions rest on the perceived credibility of the child. Black’s Law Dictionary defines credibility as “worthiness of belief” (p. 366). Although this definition is wholly unsatisfactory to research psychologists, it captures the amorphous character of credibility. “Using their experiences with human affairs,” the trier of fact is charged with the task of deciding if some, part, or all of a witness’s testimony is worthy of belief (R. v. Beland, 1987, p. 418).

An overall evaluation of perceived credibility involves two constructs: perceived honesty and perceived cognitive ability (Bottoms, 1993; Ross, Jurden, Lindsay, & Keeney, 2003). The relative importance of honesty and cognitive competence in the overall assessment of credibility is central to understanding when younger children will be seen as more or less credible than older children and adults. In a nutshell, when the abilities to encode, store, and retrieve details do not require sophisticated cognitive skills, honesty will be more important, and younger children will probably be seen as more credible than older children and adults. When encoding, storing, or reporting details requires more advanced cognitive abilities, cognitive competence will be more important, and younger children will likely be viewed as less credible than older children and adults. The analysis rests on the level of cognitive sophistication required to accurately remember a past event. When the event occurs in a familiar location and involves familiar people and/or predictable behaviours (as child abuse often does), the level of cognitive sophistication needed to encode, store, and retrieve details of the event is relatively low; if a witness is attempting to be honest, that witness will probably be accurate, at least on central details. On the other hand, if the crime occurred in an unfamiliar context and forensically relevant details are not easily remembered (e.g., a licence plate number, the colour of the light when the car went through the intersection, exactly what the bank robber said), cognitive competence will be more important, and younger children will be seen as less credible than older children and adults.

When honesty is more important, why are younger children viewed more positively than older children and adults? This is due, in large part, to (a) children’s perceived impoverished cognitive sophistication and consequent inability to fabricate a convincing lie concerning events about which they have little knowledge (i.e., sex; Bottoms, 1993), and (b) children’s perceived innate innocence. Indeed, lying is cognitively complex, and the ability to lie convincingly develops throughout childhood (e.g., Peterson, Peterson, & Seeto, 1983). Finally, child victims are seen as innocent and vulnerable (e.g., Meyer, 2007).

In many European countries, an instrument called statement validity analysis (SVA) “is probably the most popular instrument for assessing the veracity of child witnesses’ testimony in trials for sexual offences” (Vrij, 2005, p. 3). SVA consists of three components: (a) a structured interview to obtain a complete statement, (b)a systematic analysis of verbal content (the criterion-based content analysis or CBCA), (c) and a validity checklist, an evaluation of the outcome of CBCA and the testing of alternative hypotheses (Raskin & Esplin, 1991; Steller & Köehnken, 1989). CBCA is the central component of SVA and consists of a set of 19 criteria, listed in Table 6.1. An individual’s statement is rated for the presence or absence of these criteria—the more of these criteria are present, the greater the credibility of the statement. These criteria are subsumed under five subscales: (a) general characteristics, (b) specific content, (c) peculiarities of content, (d) motivationrelated content, and (e) offence-specific details (Raskin & Esplin, 1991; Steller & Köehnken, 1989).

 

Table 6.1 CBCA Criteria
General Characteristics
  1. Logical structure
  2. Unstructured production
  3. Quantity of detail
Specific Content
  1.  Contextual embedding
  2.  Description of interactions
  3. Reproduction of conversations
  4. Unexpected complications
 Peculiarities of Content
  1. Unusual details
  2. Superfluous details
  3. Accurately reported details misunderstood
  4.  Related external associations
  5. Accounts of subjective states
  6. Attributions of perpetrator’s mental state
Motivation
  1.  Spontaneous corrections
  2.  Admitting lack of memory
  3.   Raising doubts
  4.  Self-deprecation
  5.  Pardoning the perpetrator
  Offence-Specific Elements
  1. Details characteristic of the offence
Source: Raskin, D. C., & Esplin, P. W. (1991). Assessment of children’s statements of sexual abuse. In J. Doris (Ed.), The suggestibility of children’s recollections (pp. 153–164). Washington, D. C.: American Psychological Association (pp. 156–157). Reprinted with permission.

 Although SVA is popular in many European countries, an expert opinion on credibility that is based on CBCA analysis is not admissible in criminal courtin Canada (R. v. Jmieff, 1994; R. v. R. J. H., 2000; R. v. S. C. H., 1995). In R. v. Marquard (1993), the Supreme Court of Canada stated that “it is a fundamental axiom of our trial process that the ultimate conclusion as to the credibility or truthfulness of a particular witness is for the trier of fact, and is not the proper subject of expert opinion. A judge or jury who simply accepts an expert’s opinion on the credibility of a witness would be abandoning its duty to itself determine the credibility of the witness. Credibility must always be the product of the judge or jury’s view of the diverse ingredients it has perceived at trial, combined with experience, logic and an intuitive sense of the matter” (para. 49).

The law is clear; in criminal courts in Canada, experts will not be allowed to render an opinion on the credibility of a child based on CBCA or any other procedure. However, experts have been permitted to testify as to their findings based on CBCA in child protection hearings (e.g., Newfoundland (Director of Child Welfare) v. B. P., 1998; Children’s Aid Society of Halifax v. PM. H., 2006; Children’s Aid Society of Inverness-Richmond v. S. S., 2009, but see Catholic Children’s Aid Society of Toronto v. T. B., 2010, for a contrary decision).

Some scholars have raised concerns about using CBCA as the basis of courtroom testimony. Based on a review of 37 studies of CBCA, Vrij (2005) concluded that credibility assessments based on this instrument are better than lay judgments; however, they are insufficiently precise to form the basis of expert testimony (see also Lamb, 1998).

An alternative approach to credibility assessment that is gaining traction is based on reality monitoring (RM) theory ( Johnson, Hashtroudi, & Lindsay, 1993; Johnson & Raye, 1981). According to this theory, memories of real and imagined events contain different information. An imagined event will contain more information about thoughts and reasoning. An event that is real, on the other hand, will contain more perceptual information (i.e., visual and auditory details, as well as details of taste, touch, and smell), more semantic information (i.e., meanings and connections), more contextual information (i.e., details relating to time and place), and more affective information (i.e., details of feelings and emotions during the event). According to RM theory, because memories of real and imagined events contain different information, reports of them will also contain different information. There are fewer studies of RM than of CBCA, but the findings are generally positive (Alonso-Quecuty, 1992; Hernandez-Fernaud & AlonsoQuecuty, 1997; Johnson, Foley, Suengas, & Raye, 1988; Sporer, 1997; Strömwall, Bengtsson, Leander, & Granhag, 2004; but see Roberts & Lamb, 2010).

Testimonial Supports

Any witness may testify with the aid of testimonial supports, to reduce the trauma of testifying in court and to help the witness to provide a full and candid account of the allegation. Some examples of testimonial supports include having the testimony presented in court on closed-circuit TV (CCTV) while the witness testifies from outside the courtroom (Criminal Code s. 486.2), testifying from behind a screen (Criminal Code s. 486.2), or testifying with a nearby support person (Criminal Code s. 486.1(1)) (Figure 6.3a and 6.3b – See Canvas). Section 715.1 of the Criminal Code allows a videotaped interview to be admitted to replace the witness’s evidence-in-chief (i.e., responding to questions from the Crown). Screens, support persons, and CCTV must be permitted if the witness is under the age of 18 years at trial and allowing the testimonial support would not interfere with the proper administration of justice. A pre-recorded videotape must be allowed if the witness was under the age of 18 at the time of the alleged offence and allowing the videotape would not interfere with the proper administration of justice.

Learning Objective 6.6

Explain how Canadian criminal courts accommodate child witnesses.

testimonial support a procedure that deviates from traditional courtroom testimony in order to reduce the trauma of testifying and to help the witness provide a full and candid account of the allegation; for example, using screens to block the complainant’s view of the accused

When videotaped evidence is tendered, the videotape must have been made “within a reasonable time of the incident,” and the witness must “adopt the contents of the statement.” A “reasonable time” is not precisely defined; in R. v. L. (D. O.) (1993), the Supreme Court of Canada directed trial judges to consider the ability of the forensic interviewers to make a videotaped statement and the effect the actual delay from the alleged crime to the videotaped statement may have had on the witness’s memory. “Adopting the contents” means that witnesses recall having made the videotape and agree that they were being honest at the time. Note that the child does not have to recall the incident itself (R. v. F. (C. C), 1997). Multiple supports may be used (R. v. Flores, 2007).

A judge has the power to deny the use of any testimonial support if using it would interfere with the proper administration of justice. The proper administration of justice could be compromised if, for instance, the defence intends to cross-examine the child with the aid of documents that will be difficult for the court to review on closed-circuit television (R. v. G. A. P., 2007), or if testimonial supports may decrease the perceived solemnity of the occasion and cause a child to be disrespectful of the court and the process (e.g., R. v. Black, 2007). In these situations, a different testimonial support may be ordered (e.g., a screen blocking the child’s view of the accused instead of CCTV), or testimonial supports may be denied.

It has been argued that testimonial supports infringe fundamental rights under the Canadian Charter of Rights and Freedoms: namely, the right to a fair trial (s. 11(d)) and the right to fundamental justice (s. 7). The Supreme Court of Canada has heard the Charter arguments, and its clear ruling has been that testimonial supports do not infringe these rights (R. v. J. Z. S., 2010; R. v. Levogiannis, 1993; R. v. L. (D. O.), 1993). Unlike in the United States, where the confrontation clause in the Sixth Amendment of the U. S. Constitution is infringed when some testimonial supports are used, in Canada the accused does not have a Charter right to face the accuser, and the Supreme Court of Canada declined to read it into the right to a fair trial.

Testimonial supports were initially introduced to help the court obtain a full and candid account of the alleged offence. Parliament accepted that the trauma associated with testifying in open court, particularly when facing the accused, could interfere with the truth-seeking function of the court because it could lead to evidence that was less than complete and candid (Bala, 1999; Hall & Sales, 2008). Reducing stress by using testimonial supports would help the discovery of truth. Importantly, testimonial supports were and continue to be allowed if their use does not interfere with the proper administration of justice.

Stress and Testifying

The use of testimonial supports for children raises questions about the stress of testifying in court. For example, is testifying in court stressful for children? If so, does the stress impair children’s ability to testify and does the stress have long-term consequences? Do testimonial supports alleviate some of the stress, and do they interfere with the administration of justice—in this case, to the accused person’s right to a fair trial? Researchers have looked into these questions.

Stress associated with testifying can occur at two distinct points: before trial (anticipatory stress) and at trial (testimonial stress). In terms of anticipatory stress, Goodman et al. (1992) studied 120 children who had alleged child abuse; 60 of them testified and 60 did not. (The children involved in the study were matched on abuse severity, age, and pre-court behavioural adjustment.) Many of the children expressed fear of the prospect of testifying and, in particular, fear of the possibility of facing the accused. Quas et al. (2005) followed up with these children 12 to 14 years later and reported that anticipatory stress was associated with mental health problems in adulthood. Virtually every other relevant study has concluded that anticipatory stress is a major problem for children around the time of trial, and it may lead to mental health problems in adulthood.

In the case of testimonial stress, there is good empirical evidence that testifying in open court and facing the accused is a significant source of stress for some children. Other sources of stress among testifiers include lack of maternal support, testifying multiple times, lack of corroboration, and lengthy cross-examination (Goodman et al., 1992). Perhaps the most potent source of stress for a child testifying in criminal court is the need to describe the allegation in the presence of the accused, particularly if the accused is known to the child (Sas, Austin, Wolfe, & Hurley, 1991; Sas, Hurley, Hatch, Malla, & Dick, 1993). As discussed in more detail below, these stressors can impair a child’s ability to provide a full and candid account of the event (Hall & Sales, 2008), and they can have a significant negative impact on short-and long-term mental health (Goodman et al., 1992; Quas et al., 2005; Sas et al., 1993).

Evidence from actual trials and experiments indicates that stress associated with testifying in court can be reduced with the aid of testimonial supports. Hamlyn, Phelps. Turtle, and Sattar (2004) reported that, in the United Kingdom, 87 percent of the children who were offered CCTV used it, and 90 percent of those children reported that it helped because they did not have to face the accused. Davies and Noon (1993) also reported that child witnesses in criminal court who testified live were judged to be more unhappy than children who testified via CCTV. Similar conclusions can be drawn from experimental studies. In Goodman et al. (1998; discussed below), children preparing to testify via CCTV reported less stress than children preparing to testify in open court. In Landström and Granhag (2010), children reported being more nervous and said that testifying was more difficult live than via video.

Administration of Justice

Researchers have also examined the question of whether the accused’s case is prejudiced when children testify with the aid of testimonial supports. Goodman et al. (1998) conducted perhaps the most ecologically valid experiment on testimonial supports. In their study, 88 6and 8-year-old children participated, individually, in a play session with a male confederate. The children each starred in a movie, during which they put on a costume and the experimenter placed stickers on the child’s bare flesh (guilty condition) or on the child’s clothes (not guilty condition). Two weeks after the play session, each child came to an actual courtroom and was told that the experimenter may have done something wrong: he should not have made a movie of stickers being put on bare skin, and a judge wanted the child to testify about what had happened. Actors played the parts of the judge, the prosecutor, and the defence lawyer. In groups of 9 to 12 persons, community members listened to the child testify either live in the courtroom or via CCTV. The trial condition affected actual accuracy in such a way that children were more accurate in the CCTV condition than in the live condition. However, the community members’ perceptions of children’s accuracy were lower in the CCTV condition than in the live condition.

The methodology in Orcutt, Goodman, Tobey, Batterman-Faunce, & Thomas (2001) was very similar. In the live condition, the child was judged to be more accurate, more attractive/intelligent, more honest, and less likely to fabricate. Importantly, mock jurors were more likely to vote to convict the accused in the live condition than in the CCTV condition. Neither of these excellent studies found that the use of CCTV had a detrimental effect on perceptions of the accused (i.e., there was no evidence that the accused looked more guilty when the child’s testimony was delivered via CCTV). In fact, testimony via CCTV may reduce the chances of a conviction by reducing perceptions of the child’s accuracy.

To explain the differences in perception of child witnesses in the two types of testimony, Landström and Granhag (2010) proposed that perceived credibility is influenced by the vividness of the report. A more vivid report is seen as more credible than a less vivid report. Two factors that make a report vivid are temporal and locational proximity. That is, a report that is closer to the viewer in time and space is seen as more believable than a report that is more spatially and temporally distant. In terms of children’s evidence, the spatial and temporal distance is greatest between live and video, intermediate between CCTV and video, and smallest between live and CCTV. To test their theory, Landström and Granhag compared ratings of children’s testimony that was viewed live, via CCTV, or via videotape. Children were viewed most favourably in the live condition, followed by the CCTV and video conditions. Consistent with the vividness hypothesis, the biggest differences were between live and video conditions, the second biggest differences were between CCTV and video conditions, the smallest differences were between live and CCTV conditions.

Another measure of vividness, arguably, is memorability. A more vivid report should be remembered better than a less vivid report. In Landström et al. (2007), mock jurors viewed children’s reports of a staged event (similar to the one described in Landström & Granhag, 2010) via CCTV or live and measured their perceptions of their memory for the child’s report and their actual memory of the report. Relative to mock jurors who viewed the child’s testimony on CCTV, those who viewed it live rated their memories to be “better,” more complete, and more detailed. When mock jurors were asked to report the child’s testimony verbatim, those in the live condition were more accurate than those in the CCTV condition.

The law in Canada recently changed to allow testimonial supports upon request for any witness under the age of 18 unless the judge finds that testimonial supports in the circumstances may interfere with the administration of justice. This is consistent with the data on testimonial support—they may lead to a more accurate report, and there is no evidence that their use prejudices the accused. In fact, use of testimonial supports may have a detrimental effect on the perceived credibility of the child.

Child Advocacy Centres

As should be clear from this chapter, the needs of child witnesses are complex and require the coordinated effort of many diverse professionals. In the past few years, the federal government has made a significant financial commitment to young victims of crime. Funding has been used to establish child advocacy centres (CACs). As of April, 2013, six CACs are fully operational in Canada, and 19 others are in various stages of development (McDonald, Scrim, & Rooney, 2013). CACs provide a multidisciplinary and integrated approach to children who are involved in the justice system. The services available may include law enforcement, child protection, prosecution, mental health support, advocacy, forensic interviewing, and medical evaluations. Not all CACs offer all of the services, but they share a single goal: to reduce the trauma that child victims and witnesses of crime experience when involved in the criminal justice system.

Summary

In the past few decades, dramatic changes have occurred in law and in psychological research that have moved us from being a country where “sexual offences involving children were virtually impossible to prosecute to conviction” (Van Tongeren Harvey & Dauns, 2001, p. 148) to being a country where children may, and often do, testify with the aid of innovative and effective testimonial supports, and where crimes against children are prosecuted to conviction. When children are interviewed in ways that are consistent with evidence-based recommendations, they can report a substantial amount of correct information about things that happened to them in the past. Children can and will report honestly, and a promise to tell the truth promotes truthful reports in children as young as four years old. However, children’s reports can be compromised when inept and/ or partisan adults are involved. When exposed to biasing influences, children may report false details of a past event or entire events that did not happen. Testifying in open court while facing the accused can be terrifying and can lead to reports that are less complete and candid than would otherwise be available. What can we take from this? Persons working with children in the criminal justice system should be well trained and should always conduct interviews in ways that are consistent with generally accepted and evidence-based practices. Failure to do this is our failure, not the failure of children.

DISCUSSION QUESTIONS

  • Is there an age at which children are too young to testify in criminal court? Explain your answer and discuss the implications of your decision. For instance, if you think children below a certain age should not testify in court, what might that mean for child victims? If you believe all children should be allowed to testify, regardless of age, what might that mean for accused persons?
  • A truth-lie discussion has not been shown to promote truth-telling behaviour in young children. However, it continues to be used in some jurisdictions and at some stages in the legal process. Beyond a truth-promoting function, can you think of any other reason to engage young children in a truth-lie discussion?
  • Prepare a script that can be used to close a forensic interview with a child.
  • According to section 486.2(1) of the Criminal Code of Canada, any witness under the age of 18, upon application by the prosecution, can testify outside the courtroom or behind a screen so as not to see the defendant. These requests are granted if the supports can help the child witness provide a “full and candid account of the acts complained of.” Given what you know about the law and what you know about children as witnesses, comment on the use of testimonial supports for children from two perspectives: the perspective of the defence and the perspective of the prosecution.
  • Canada has recently provided substantial funding for child advocacy centres (CAC). If you were a director of one of the new centres and your budget allowed you to respond to three issues concerning child witnesses, what three issues would you address and why?

 

 

Key Terms

closed prompts

commission errors

competence

credibility

omission errors

open prompts

recent complaint doctrine

children’s suggestibility

testimonial support

truth-lie discussion

 

References

Alonso-Quecuty, M. L. (1992). Deception detection and reality monitoring: A new answer to an old question? In F. Lösel, D. Bender, & T. Bliesener (Eds.), Psychology and law: International perspectives (pp. 328–332). Berlin: Walter de Gruyter.

Badgley Report. (1984). Sexual offences against children: Report of the Committee on Sexual Offences against Children and Youth. Vols. 1 and 2. Ottawa: Department of Supply and Services.

Bala, N. (1990). Double victims: Child sexual abuse and the Canadian criminal justice system. Queen’s Law Journal, 15, 3–32.

Bala, N. (1999). Child witnesses in the Canadian criminal courts. Psychology, Public Policy, & Law, 5, 323–354.

Bala, N., Paetsch, J. J., Bertrand, L. D., & Thomas, M. (2010). Testimonial support provisions for children and vulnerable adults (Bill C-2): Case law review and perceptions of the judiciary. Retrieved from www.justice.gc.ca/eng/pi/ rs/rep-rap/2010/rr10_vic3/rr10_vic3.pdf.

Black’s Law Dictionary, 6th ed. (1990) Eagan, MN: West Group.

Blandon-Gitlan, I., Pezdek, K., Rogers, M., & Brodie, L. (2005). Detecting deception in children: An experimental study of the effect of event familiarity on CBCA Ratings. Law and Human Behavior, 29, 187–197.

Bottoms, B. L. (1993). Individual differences in perceptions of child sexual assault victims. In G. S. Goodman & B. L. Bottoms (Eds.), Child victims, child witnesses: Understanding and improving testimony (pp. 229–261). New York, NY: Guilford. Bottoms, B. L., Quas, J. A., & Davis, S. L. (2007). The influence of interviewer-provided social support on children’s suggestibility, memory, and disclosures. In M. E. Pipe, M. Lamb, Y. Orbach, & A. C. Cederborg (Eds.), Child sexual abuse: Disclosure, delay and denial (pp. 135–158). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.

Brainerd, C. J., & Reyna, V. F. (2012). Reliability of children’s testimony in the era of developmental reversals. Developmental Review, 32, 224–267.

Brainerd, C. J., Reyna, V. F., & Forrest, T. J. (2002). Are young children susceptible to the false-memory illusion? Child Development, 73, 1363–1377.

Brodie v. The King, [1936] S. C. R. 188.

Brubacher, S. P., Roberts, K. P., & Powell, M. (2011). Effects of practicing episodic versus scripted recall on children’s subsequent narratives of a repeated event. Psychology, Public Policy, and Law, 17, 286–314.

Carter, C. A., Bottoms, B. L., & Levine, M. (1996). Linguistic and socioemotional influences on the accuracy of children’s reports. Law and Human Behavior, 20, 335–358.

Catholic Children’s Aid Society of Toronto v. T. B. [2010] O. J. No. 590. (Ontario Court of Justice).

Ceci S. J., & Bruck, M. (1993). The suggestibility of children’s recollections: An historical review and synthesis. Psychological Bulletin, 113, 403–439.

Ceci, S. J., & Bruck, M. (1995). Jeopardy in the courtroom: A scientific analysis of children’s testimony. Washington, D. C.: American Psychological Association. Ceci, S. J., & Bruck, M. (2006). Children’s suggestibility: Characteristics and mechanisms. In R. V. Kail (Ed.), Advances in child development and behavior

(pp. 247–281). San Diego, CA: Elsevier Academic Press.

Ceci, S. J., Loftus, E. F., Leichtman, M. D., & Bruck, M. (1994). The possible role of source misattributions in the creation of false beliefs among preschoolers. International Journal of Clinical and Experimental Hypnosis, 42, 304–320.

Charter of Rights and Freedoms, Part I of the Constitution Act, 1982, being Schedule B to the Canada Act, 1982 (U.K.), 1982, c. 11.Children’s Aid Society of Halifax v. PM. H. [2006] N. S. J. No. 165. (Supreme Court).

Children’s Aid Society of Inverness-Richmond v. S. S. [2009] N. S. J. No 226. (Supreme Court).

Connolly, D. A., & Lindsay, D. S. (2001). The influence of suggestions on children’s reports of a unique experience versus an instance of a repeated experience. Applied Cognitive Psychology, 15, 205–223.

Connolly, D. A., & Price, H. L. (2006). Children’s suggestibility for an instance of a repeated event versus a unique event: The effect of degree of association between variable options. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 93, 207–223. Connolly, D. A., Price, H. L., & Gordon, H. M. (2009). Judging the credibility of historic child sexual abuse complainants: How judges describe their decisions. Psychology, Public Policy, and Law, 15, 102–123.

Connolly, D. A., Price, H. L., Lavoie, J. A., & Gordon, H. M. (2008). Perceptions and predictors of children’s credibility of a unique event and an instance of a repeated event. Law and Human Behavior, 32, 92–112.

Connolly, D. A., & Read, J. D. (2006). Delayed prosecutions of historic child sexual abuse: Analyses of 2064 Canadian criminal complaints. Law and Human Behavior, 30, 409–434.

Criminal Code, R. S. C. 1985, c. C.46.

Davies, G., & Noon, E. (1993). Video links: Their impact on child witness trials. Issues in Criminological & Legal Psychology, 20, 22–26.

Davis, S. L., & Bottoms, B. L. (2002). Effects of social support on children’s eyewitness reports: A test of the underlying mechanism. Law and Human Behavior, 26, 185–215.

Evans, A. D., & Lee. K. (2010). Promising to tell the truth makes 8to 16-yearolds more honest. Behavioral Sciences and the Law, 28, 801–811.

Fivush, R. (2002). Scripts, schemas, and memory of trauma. In N. L. Stein,

P. J. Bauer, & M. Rabinowitz (Eds.), Representation, memory, and development: Essays in honor of Jean Mandler (pp. 53–74). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.

Garven, S., Wood, J. M., & Malpass, R. S. (2000). Allegations of wrongdoing: The effects of reinforcement on children’s mundane and fantastic claims. Journal of Applied Psychology, 85, 38–49.

Gee, S., Gregory, M., & Pipe, M. E. (1999). What colour is your pet dinosaur? The impact of pre-interview training and question type on children’s answers. Legal and Criminological Psychology, 4, 111–128.

Goodman, G. S., Batterman-Faunce, J., Schaaf, J. M., & Kenney, R. (2002). Nearly 4 years after an event: Children’s eyewitness memory and adults’ perceptions of children’s accuracy. Child Abuse & Neglect, 26, 849–88.

Goodman, G. S., Sharma, A., Thomas, S. F., & Considine, M. G. (1995). Mother knows best: Effects of relationship status and interviewer bias on children’s memory. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 60, 195–228.

Goodman, G. S., Taub, E. P., Jones, D. P., England, P., Port, L. K., Rudy, L., & Prado, L. (1992). Testifying in criminal court: Emotional effects on child sexual assault victims. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development, 57, v–142.

Goodman, G. S., Tobey, A. E., Batterman-Faunce, J. M., Orcutt, H., Thomas, S., Shapiro, C., & Sachsenmaier, T. (1998). Face-to-face confrontation: Effects of closed-circuit technology on children’s eyewitness testimony and jurors’ decisions. Law and Human Behavior, 22, 165–203.

Guadagno, B. L., Powell, M., & Wright, R. (2006) Police officers’ and legal professionals’ perceptions regarding how children are, and should be, questioned about repeated abuse. Psychiatry, Psychology, and Law, 13, 251–260.

Hall, S. R., & Sales, B. D. (2008). Courtroom modifications for child witnesses: Law and science in forensic evaluations. Washington, D. C.: American Psychological Association.

Hamlyn, B., Phelps, A., Turtle, J., & Sattar, G. (2004). Are special measures working? Evidence from surveys of vulnerable and intimidated witnesses. Retrieved February 27, 2012, from http://library.npia.police.uk/docs/hors/hors283.pdf.

Hernandez-Fernaud, E., & Alonso-Quecuty, M. L. (1997). The cognitive interview and lie detection: A new magnifying glass for Sherlock Holmes? Applied Cognitive Psychology, 11, 55–68.

Hershkowitz, I. (2001). Children’s responses to open-ended utterances in investigative interviews. Legal and Criminological Psychology, 6, 49–63.

Hershkowitz, I. (2002). The role of facilitative prompts in interviews of alleged sex and abuse victims. Legal and Criminological Psychology, 7, 63–71.

Hudson, J. A., & Mayhew, E. Y. (2009). The development of memory for recurring events. In M. L. Courage & N. Cowan (Eds.), The development of memory in infancy and childhood (2nd ed.) (pp. 69–91). New York, NY: Psychology Press. Johnson, M. K., Foley, M. A., Suengas, A. G., & Raye, C. L. (1988). Phenomenal characteristics of memories for perceived and imagined autobiographical events. Journal of Experimental Psychology General, 117, 371–376.

Johnson, M. K., Hashtroudi, S., & Lindsay, D. S. (1993). Source monitoring. Psychological Bulletin, 114, 3–28.

Johnson, M. K., & Raye, C. L. (1981). Reality monitoring. Psychological Review, 88, 67–85.

Jones, D. P. H., & Krugman, R. D. (1986). Can a three-year-old bear witness to her sexual assault and attempted murder? Child Abuse & Neglect, 10, 253–258.

Lamb, M. E. (1998). Assessments of children’s credibility in forensic contexts.

Current Directions in Psychological Science, 7, 43–46.

Lamb, M. E., Orbach, Y., Hershkowitz, I., Esplin, P. W., & Horowitz, D. (2007). A structured forensic interview protocol improves the quality and informativeness of investigative interviews with children: A review of research using the NICHD Investigative Interview Protocol. Child Abuse & Neglect, 31, 1201–1231.

Landström, S., Granhag, P. A., & Hartwig, M. (2007). Children’s live and videotaped testimonies: How presentation mode affects observers’ perception, assessment and memory. Legal and Criminological Psychology, 12, 333–347.

Landström, S., & Granhag, P. (2010). In-court versus out-of-court testimonies: Children’s experiences and adults’ assessments. Applied Cognitive Psychology, 24, 941–955.

Leichtman, M. D., & Ceci, S. J. (1995). The effects of stereotypes and suggestions on preschoolers’ reports. Developmental Psychology, 31, 568–578.

Lewis, P. (2006). Delayed prosecution for childhood sexual abuse. New York: Oxford University Press.

London, K., Bruck, M., Ceci, S. J., & Shuman, D. W. (2005). Disclosure of child sexual abuse: What does the research tell us about the ways that children tell? Psychology, Public Policy, and Law, 11, 194–226.

Lyon, T. D., & Dorado, J. S. (2008). Truth induction in young maltreated children: The effects of oath-taking and reassurance on true and false disclosures. Child Abuse & Neglect, 32, 738–748.

Lyon, T. D., Saywitz, K. J., Kaplan, D. L., & Dorado, J. S. (2001). Reducing maltreated children’s reluctance to answer hypothetical oath-taking competency questions. Law and Human Behavior, 25, 81–92.

McDonald, S., Scrim, K., & Rooney, L. (2013). Building our capacity: Children’s Advocacy Centres in Canada. Retrieved from Department of Justice website, http://canada.justice.gc.ca/eng/rp-pr/cj-jp/victim/rd6-rr6/rd6-rr6.pdf.

Meyer, A. (2007). The moral rhetoric of childhood. Childhood, 14, 85–104. Newfoundland (Director of Child Welfare) v. B. P. [1998] N. J. No. 139. (Supreme

Court).

Orbach, Y., & Lamb, M. E. (1999). Assessing the accuracy of a child’s account of sexual abuse: A case study. Child Abuse & Neglect, 23, 91–98.

Orcutt, H. K., Goodman, G. S., Tobey, A. E., Batterman-Faunce, J. M., & Thomas, S. (2001). Detecting deception in children’s testimony: Factfinders’ abilities to reach the truth in open court and closed-circuit trials. Law and Human Behavior, 25, 339–372.

Perona, A. R., Bottoms, B. L., & Sorenson, E. (2006). Research-based guidelines for child forensic interviews. Journal of Aggression, Maltreatment & Trauma, 12(3–4), 81–130.

Peterson, C. C., Peterson, J. L., & Seeto, D. (1983). Developmental changes in the ideas about lying. Child Development, 54, 1529–1535.

Pezdek, K., Morrow, A., Blandon-Gitlin, I., Goodman, G. S., Quas, J. A., Saywitz, K. J. , . . . Brodie, L. (2004). Deception detection in children: Event familiarity affects criterion-based content analysis ratings. Journal of Applied Psychology, 89, 119–126.

Poole. D. A., & Lamb, M. E. (2002). Investigative interviews of children. Washington,

D. C.: American Psychological Association.

Powell, M. B., & Roberts, K. P. (2002). The effect of repeated experience on children’s suggestibility across two question types. Applied Cognitive Psychology, 16, 367–386.

Price, H. L., & Connolly, D. A. (2004). Event frequency and children’s suggestibility: A study of cued recall responses. Applied Cognitive Psychology, 18(7), 809–821.

Principe, G. F., & Ceci, S. J. (2002). “I saw it with my own ears”: The effects of peer conversation on preschoolers’ reports of nonexperienced events. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 83, 1–25.

Principe, G. F., DiPuppo, J., & Gammel, J. (2011, April). Leaking misinformation to mothers: Effects of maternal conversational style on young children’s suggestibility for a past event. Poster presented at the meetings of the Society for Research in Child Development, Montreal, Quebec, Canada.

Principe, G. F., & Schindewolf, E. (2012). Natural conversations as a source of false memories in children: Implications for the testimony of young witnesses. Developmental Review, 32, 205–223.

Quas, J. A., Goodman, G. S., Ghetti, S., Alexander, K. W., Edelstein, R., Redlich, A. D. , . . . Jones, D. P. (2005). Childhood sexual assault victims: Long-term outcomes after testifying in criminal court. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development, 70, 1–145.

R. v. Beland ([1987] 2 S. C. R. 398.

R. v. Black, [2007] B. C. J. 2035 (S. C.).

R. v. D. D., [2000] S. C. J. No. 44.

R. v. F. (C. C.), [1997] 3 S. C. R. 1183.

R. v. Flores, [2007] B. C. J. 1505 (S. C.).

R. v. G. A. P., [2007] M. J. No. 194 (Man. Q. B.).

R. v. J. Z. S., [2010] 1 S. C. R. 3.

R. v. Jmieff, [1994] B. C. J. No. 2506 (C. A.).

R. v. L. (D. O.) (1993), 85 C. C. C. (3d) 289 (S. C. C.).

R. v. Levogiannis, [1993] 4 S. C. R. 475.

R. v. Marquard, [1993] S. C. J. No. 119.

R. v. R. J. H. [2000] B. C. J. No. 1493. (S. C.).

R. v. S. C. H. [1995] B. C. J. No. 237. (S. C. ).

R. v. Sterling, [1995] S. J. No. 612 (C. A.).

Raskin, D. C., & Esplin, P. W. (1991). Assessment of children’s statements of sexual abuse. In J. Doris (Ed.), The suggestibility of children’s recollections (pp. 153–164). Washington, D. C.: American Psychological Association.

Roberts, K. P., & Lamb, M. E. (2010). Reality-monitoring characteristics in confirmed and doubtful allegations of child sexual assault. Applied Cognitive Psychology, 24, 1049–1079.

Roberts, K. P., Lamb, M. E., & Sternberg, K. J. (2004). The effects of rapportbuilding style on children’s reports of a staged event. Applied Cognitive Psychology, 18, 189–202.

Roberts, K. P., & Powell, M. B. (2006). The consistency of false suggestions moderates children’s reports of a single instance of a repeated event: Predicting increases and decreases in suggestibility. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 94, 68–89.

Ross, D. F., Jurden, F. H., Lindsay, R. L., & Keeney, J. M. (2003). Replications and limitations of a two-factor model of child witness credibility. Journal of Applied Social Psychology, 33, 418–430.

Sas, L. D. (1993). Three years after the verdict: A longitudinal study of the social and psychological adjustment of child witnesses referred to the Child Witness Project. London, ON: London Family Court Clinic.

Sas, L. D., Austin, G., Wolfe, D., & Hurley, P. (1991). Reducing the system induced trauma for child sexual abuse victims through court preparation, assessment, and follow-up. London, ON: London Family Court Clinic.

Sas, L. D., & Cunningham, A. H. (1995). Tipping the balance to tell the secret: Public discovery of child sexual abuse. London, ON: London Family Court Clinic.

Sas, L. D., Hurley, P., Hatch, A., Malla, S., & Dick, T. (1993). Three years after the verdict: A longitudinal study of the social and psychological adjustment of child witnesses referred to the child witness project. Ottawa, ON: Health and Welfare Canada.

Saywitz, K. J., & Camparo, L. B. (2009). Contemporary child forensic interviewing: Evolving consensus and innovation over 25 years. In B. L. Bottoms,

C. J. Najdowski, & G. S. Goodman (Eds.), Children as victims, witnesses, and offenders: Psychological science and the law. New York, NY: Guilford.

Saywitz, K. J., & Moan-Hardie, S. (1994). Reducing the potential for distortion of childhood memories. Consciousness and Cognition: An International Journal, 3, 408–425.

Sporer, S. L. (1997). The less traveled road to truth: Verbal cues in deception detection in accounts of fabricated and self-experienced events. Applied Cognitive Psychology, 11, 373–397.

Steller, M., & Köehnken, G. (1989). Criteria-based statement analysis. In D. C. Raskin, (Ed.), Psychological methods in criminal investigation and evidence (pp. 217–245). New York, NY: Springer.

Sternberg, K. J., Lamb, M. E., Hershkowitz, I., Yudilevitch, L., Orbach, Y., Esplin,

P. W., & Hovav, M. (1997). Effects of introductory style on children’s abilities to describe experiences of sexual abuse. Child Abuse & Neglect, 21, 1133–1146. Strömwall, L. A., Bengtsson, L., Leander, L., & Granhag, P. A. (2004). Assessing children’s statements: The impact of a repeated experience on CBCA and

RM ratings. Applied Cognitive Psychology, 18, 653–668.

Talwar, V., Lee, K., Bala, N., & Lindsay, R. L. (2002). Children’s conceptual knowledge of lying and its relation to their actual behaviors: Implications for court competence examinations. Law and Human Behavior, 26, 395–415.

Talwar, V., Lee, K., Bala, N., & Lindsay, R. L. (2004). Children’s lie-telling to conceal a parent’s transgression: Legal implications. Law and Human Behavior, 28, 411–435.

Teoh, Y., & Lamb, M. E. (2010). Preparing children for investigative interviews: Rapport-building, instruction, and evaluation. Applied Developmental Science, 14, 154–163.

Teoh, Y., & Lamb, M. E. (2013). Interviewer demeanor in forensic interviews of children. Psychology, Crime, and Law, 19, 145–159.

Thompson, W. C., Clarke-Stewart, K. A., & Lepore, S. J. (1997). What did the janitor do? Suggestive interviewing and the accuracy of children’s accounts. Law and Human Behavior, 21, 405–426.

Van Tongeren, H. W., & Dauns, P. E. (2001). Sexual offences against children and the criminal process (2nd ed.). Markham, ON: Butterworth.

Vrij, A. (2005). Criteria-based content analysis: A qualitative review of the first 37 studies. Psychology, Public Policy, & Law, 11, 3–41.

White, T. L., Leichtman, M. D., & Ceci, S. J. (1997). The good, the bad, and the ugly: Accuracy, inaccuracy, and elaboration in preschoolers’ reports about a past event. Applied Cognitive Psychology, 11 (Special Issue), S37-S54.

Wood, J. M., & Garven, S. (2000). How sexual abuse interviews go astray: Implications for prosecutors, police, and child protection services. Child Maltreatment, 5, 109–118.

Suggested Readings and Websites

Bala, N., Lindsay, R. C. L., & McNamara, E. (2001). Testimonial aids of children: The Canadian experience with closed circuit television, screens and videotapes. Criminal Law Quarterly, 44, 461–486.

Ceci, S. J., & Bruck, M. (2007). Loftus’s lineage in developmental forensic research: Six scientific misconceptions about children’s suggestibility. In

M. Garry & H. Hayne (Eds.), Do justice and let the sky fall: Elizabeth Loftus and her contributions to science, law, and academic freedom (pp. 65–77). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.

Department of Justice, Canada. (2011). Child and youth victims and witnesses of crime in Canada. Retrieved from www.orcacentre.ca/research-a-information

/reports-and-discussion-papers.

Centre for Children and Families in the Justice System: www.lfcc.on.ca. Child and Youth Advocacy Centres: http://cac-cae.ca/.

 

License

Share This Book